Skip to main content

A study of carpenter apprentices’ spatial skills

Abstract

Background

Spatial skills are crucial for carpentry and are a major learning objective in the initial vocational training of carpenter apprentices. Carpenters specifically need to develop the capability to switch between two-dimensional (2D) and three-dimensional (3D) representations. Previous studies have explored spatial skills, but never in the context of vocational education and training (VET). This study sheds light on the level and evolution of spatial skills in the initial vocational training of carpenter apprentices in Switzerland.

Methods

In this study, 726 subjects (98 females) who were either carpenter apprentices, apprentices of another profession, or high school students, took a test on spatial skills with three parts: mental rotation, paper folding, and orthographic projections. The first two parts are widely used tests for spatial skills, while the last one was specifically designed to address the 2D-3D transition that is a core skill of carpenters.

Results

Carpenter apprentices do have higher spatial skills than would be expected given their general school level. In particular, their spatial skills were found to be similar to those of high school students and superior to those of apprentices of another profession. Carpenters’ spatial skills improve over the course of their apprenticeship. These findings confirm that spatial skills are trainable and suggest that the high spatial skills level of carpenter apprentices is due to a selection bias as well as to the training that they receive during their apprenticeship.

Conclusions

Carpenter apprentices improve their spatial skills over the time of their initial vocational training. As spatial skills are crucial in this profession, there is a need to develop further solutions that encourage further improvement of teaching and learning activities for spatial skills.

Background

The study reported in this paper was conducted as part of a Swiss research initiative that aims at supporting initial vocational education and training (VET) by means of technologies. The initiative explores and evaluates the benefits of tangible user interfaces (TUIs) to help carpenter apprentices learn their trade. TUIs are special kinds of computer interfaces that allow the user to control the computer through the manipulation of physical objects (Ishii and Ullmer [15]). The rationale behind using TUIs in the context of VET is that they are allow apprentices to continue to learn in a similar way as they currently do, that is with concrete learning material and hands-on activities (e.g. Manches and O’ Malley [20]; Do-Lenh et al. [6]).

The aim of the first study within the project was to get familiar with the competences apprentices need and to inform the design of a tangible learning environment for carpenters. The results of a contextual inquiry in the apprentices schools and workplaces indicated that spatial skills should be the main focus of the project. The literature on spatial skills and vocational training in general is scarce, even though some studies have shown that the practitioners’ approach to solving practical spatial tasks differs from that of students (Jurdak and Shahin [16]). This gap needs to be filled, as spatial skills are key in many vocationally trained professions (brick layers, metalworker, furniture maker, draftsman, etc.). This study is a first step in contributing data on spatial skills in VET. It does so by comparing carpenter apprentices’ spatial skills to other similar populations, and by following their spatial skills development in a longitudinal approach.

The results of this study are of interest for three audiences: (1) it provides carpentry teachers and VET-focused instructional designers information about the current skill level and skill development of carpenter apprentices; (2) it provides new and original data on an often neglected segment of the population (apprentices) to researchers interested in spatial skills; and (3) it helps identify the specific needs for acquiring spatial skills, which in turn informs the design of the tangible learning environment.

The remainder of this introduction will illustrate how carpenters in general are trained, elucidate the contextual inquiry among carpentry professionals, and present an overview of existing literature on spatial skills research, before outlining the hypotheses addressed in this study.

Carpenters in Switzerland

In the Swiss vocational system, there are two different training paths: one for construction carpentry (“charpentier” in French, “Zimmermann” in German), and one for furniture making (“menuisier”, “Schreiner”). This article focuses on construction carpenters, who construct large items, such as roof structures or even entire buildings (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Structures built by carpenters. Examples of such structures.

In 2010, with 1060 apprentices starting an apprenticeship, carpentry was the 19th most popular profession by the number of apprentices starting an apprenticeship (Gaillard [8]). In 2011, 890 federal degrees for carpenters were delivered (869 men and 21 women) and there were 2887 people (29 women) enrolled in a carpenter apprenticeship, representing about 10% of all apprentices in the building trades.

The job of a carpenter is to prepare, cut, and assemble wood pieces to create the frames and roofs of buildings. Carpenters work on new buildings as well as on older ones that they renovate. Their job mainly consists of the following five steps:

  1. 1.

    Read and make sense of the plans produced by the architect (or the engineer).

  2. 2.

    Generate a working drawing based on the plans, at a 1:1 scale.

  3. 3.

    Determine how much wood will be needed, select and prepare it.

  4. 4.

    Cut the various pieces of wood as indicated on the plan.

  5. 5.

    Go to the construction site and assemble all the pieces together.

For all the steps but the third one, spatial skills play a key role. At the end of the apprenticeship, a carpenter must be able to perform all these steps correctly.

The carpenter apprenticeship currently lasts for three years (it will be extended by one year, starting in 2014). Following the general VET scheme, the training happens in 3 places: the professional school (1 day a week), the company (4 days a week), and the inter-company courses, a few weeks per year for a total of 32 days over the 3 years.

Contextual inquiry in professional schools and companies

In preparation to this study, visits to the schools and to the companies revealed the different perspective that stakeholders from professional schools and companies have on how to train professionals.

Overall, company directors and teachers from professional schools agreed on one point: being a carpenter requires excellent spatial skills. Indeed the information regarding the physical, 3D items that carpenters have to build is conveyed by means of 2D paper plans. Tasks such as transitioning from the paper plans to the final object and imagining how several beams are going to fit together are carpenters’ bread-and-butter and require excellent spatial skills.

From the visits at the schools as well as the studies of the learning material provided to carpenter apprentices, we established that about 60% of the profession-specific teaching time is dedicated to drawing. This includes activities such as learning the basics of descriptive geometry and how to draw carpentry plans. The remaining 40% are split between learning the required mathematics subjects applied to carpentry as well as studying physics building, building materials, and structural mechanics.

However, in light of this learning curriculum, the bottom line from the company stakeholders’ was that schools had not adapted to the changes that the profession had undergone With the recent advent of new technology such as computer-aided design (CAD) software and computerized numerical control (CNC), and with the new construction standards, the needs for training of new carpenters had drastically changed. According to them, the need to learn drawing for new generations of carpenters is much weaker than it was before.

When interviewed at work and at school, apprentices confirmed that, in the workplace, they almost never draw anything, since most of the plans are made by their supervisor, be it with a CAD software or by hand. While not all of them disliked drawing, most of them said they did not understand why they have to spend so much time learning how to draw when drawing will most likely never be part of their work tasks.

The teachers, on the other hand, had a different take on the subject: they insisted on the fact that drawing was the basis of the profession and that it should definitely not be abandoned. They acknowledged that drawing was not per se used in the professional environment anymore, but emphasized that it was key to learn the concepts of the profession, helped apprentices learn to read plans, and helped develop their spatial skills.

Overview of spatial skills

Definition of spatial skills

Spatial cognition addresses how humans acquire and encode spatial information, and how it is represented in memory and manipulated internally (Quarles et al. [23]; Hegarty et al. [14]). It is a specific and important kind of cognition, to the point that Gardner ([9]), in his original theory on multiple intelligences, identified spatial intelligence as one of the seven types of intelligence that humans possess.

Although various classifications of spatial skills have been proposed, a separation into two main dimensions is usually accepted: visualization (e.g. mental rotation, paper folding) and navigation (e.g. map orientation). These two dimensions have also been defined as “scales” of spatial ability (Hegarty et al. [14]), with visualization being considered small-scale and navigation large-scale. Capability in each of these two dimensions may be independent. Uttal et al. ([31]) have defined four other classifications that are based on the type of information that a task requires (extrinsic versus intrinsic) and whether it is dynamic or static. For example, a mental rotation task would be classified as dynamic (one has to mentally turn the object) and intrinsic, since it requires a comparison of the parts of an object, as opposed to a comparison of the object with its environment.

For carpenters, while multiple types of spatial skills are certainly useful, the most needed one is visualization (or intrinsic, both static and dynamic). This is the skill that allows them to go back and forth between the 2D representation of an object, given by the plan, and the 3D object itself. With good spatial skills, they can imagine, based on a plan, how a building will look like, how beams will be assembled, or check on a plan whether what they built is correct.

In the rest of article, “spatial skills” refers to the spatial visualization skills that carpenters use to imagine a 3D object from a 2D paper representation, and the other way around. For carpenters, the 2D representation is given by orthographic projections, which are a kind of parallel projection where all projection lines are orthogonal to the projection plane. Figure 2 shows the orthographic projections and a perspective 3D representation of it. The 3D object can be mentally elaborated by cross-checking information on the 3 orthographic projections if there are no coinciding projections of vertices (Lafue [18]).

Figure 2
figure 2

Orthographic projections.

Importance of spatial skills

As pointed out by Sorby ([28]), “spatial skills have been a significant area of research in educational psychology since the 1920s or 30s”. Educational systems, though, have often neglected and looked down on visual thinking, favoring instead other cognitive skills (Sommer [27], Arnheim [1]; Smith [26]; Gardner [9]). In the last decades, the interest for spatial skills has increased, leading to a growing body of research on understanding, assessing, and developing means of training spatial skills.

In a study involving 400,000 participants of grades 9 to 12 over 11 years, Wai et al. ([33]) measured the link between spatial capability and STEM domains (science, technology, engineering, and mathematics). Their results showed that spatial capability has a strong influence on the development of expertise in STEM fields. For example, individuals holding at least a Bachelor’s degree in engineering have spatial skills that are more than one-and-a-half standard deviation higher than the rest of the population. Other researchers also suggested that spatial capability assessment could be used to improve the detection of teenager talents for the STEM domains (Shea et al. [24]), or that improving mathematics and science skills could be achieved by enhancing spatial thinking (Newcombe [21]; Sorby et al. [29]). Another possibility might be that improving spatial skills could reduce the number of university dropouts in engineering studies (e.g. (Sorby [28])).

Spatial skills are not only important for school subjects. In the professional world, spatial visualization skills and mental rotation capabilities are important for technical professions (Maier [19]). The professions that come first to mind are those that require a high level in STEM domains, such as mechanical engineer and architect. However, there are also some more unexpected professions for which spatial skills play an important role. For instance, Hegarty et al. ([13]) showed that dentistry students develop mental models of teeth. Hambrick et al. ([11]) found that novice geologists’ spatial capability was correlated with their score at geology mapping tasks and concluded that specifically training spatial capability could benefit especially young professionals. Several studies showed the importance of spatial thinking for chemical sciences (e.g. Carter et al. ([2])). Hamlin et al. ([12]) showed that a person’s spatial capability influences his or her capability to learn and use 3D modeling software.

For a long time, there was a wide spread belief that one is born with a given potential for spatial thinking and that this potential is fixed. In fact, skepticism about the malleability of spatial skills was persistent for many years even among researchers, with many of them arguing that training spatial skills only leads to short improvements, and only in cases where the training and measurement tasks are very similar (e.g. (Sims and Mayer [25])). However, in the most recent and comprehensive meta-analysis to date on spatial skills training, (Uttal et al. ([31]), p. 27) found that spatial skills were “moderately malleable and that training, on average, improved performance by almost half a standard deviation”.

Goal of this study

The importance of spatial skills reported by both the carpentry teachers and the directors of carpentry companies is coherent with the finding that technical professions require some well-developed spatial skills. The literature, however, provides mainly insights on spatial skills of high school and university students. We lack knowledge about spatial skills of apprentices in initial vocational training and even more so of specifically carpenter apprentices. This study pursues the goal of finding (1) whether carpenter apprentices indeed have well-developed spatial skills compared to other populations, and (2) whether their spatial skills improve throughout their initial vocational training. Formally, this means that we have two hypotheses:

The spatial skills of carpenter apprentices are higher than those of same age individuals that do not attend a carpentry training.

The spatial skills of carpenter apprentices improve over the course of their apprenticeship.

Methods

To verify these two hypotheses, the first step was to create testing material (see next paragraph). Answering H A required comparison populations. Two target populations, besides carpenters, were chosen so as to represent various samples of the population: apprentices of another profession (logistics apprentices with whom we had worked in a previous research project), and more academically oriented subjects (high school students). The results concerning H A are discussed in the first part of the results section.

As for H B , there are two ways to test it: compare the performance of apprentices enrolled in different years of the apprenticeship, or compare the performance of the same apprentices several times over time. The results of both approaches are reported in the second part of the results section.

Testing material

There exist several tests to measure spatial skills. The one used in this study consists of three parts: mental rotation (MR), paper folding (PF), and orthographic projection (OP). The MR and PF parts are taken from two widely used tests to measure mental rotation and spatial visualization capabilities, respectively (Peters et al. [22]; Ekstrom et al. [7]), these being the main tests used in the literature. The mental rotation test comprises of two series of 12 questions, and the paper folding test is made of two series of 10 questions. The MR and PF tests were chosen for this study because they are the main tests used in the literature. The orthographic projection part was designed specifically for this test and is composed of 6 questions that require the participant to match a 3D perspective view of an object with a 2D one. These additional items were added so that the test be closer to the practice of carpenters.

Example questions of each of the three parts are shown in Figure 3. For the mental rotation test, two of the four figures on the right are a vertical rotation of the left image (Figure 3A). The participant must identify these two figures. For the paper folding test, a square piece of paper is folded and then punched as shown on the left (Figure 3B). The participant must find which one of the five figures on the right matches the piece of paper, once it is unfolded. For the orthographic projections, there are two types of questions. In the first type (Figure 3C), the participant must tell which one of the four 3D models matches the three orthographic projections. In the second type (Figure 3D), a perspective representation of the model is shown and the subjects must determine which one of the four 2D drawing matches the 3D model when seen as shown by the arrow.

Figure 3
figure 3

Samples test questions. Example questions for the three parts of the test used to assess spatial skills: mental rotation (A), paper folding (B), and orthographic projections (C and D).

Participants

A total of 726 subjects (98 females) were tested. The subjects were either carpenter apprentices, logistics apprentices, or high school students. Their age ranged between 14 and 40 years, with an average age of 18. Tables 1 and 2 summarize the data by year and by gender for the three separate types of curriculum. The duration of each curriculum is three years, but practical constraints allowed us to gather third year data for carpenters only. For the years 1, 2, and 3, the tests were taken at the very end of the academic year, meaning that year 1 students already had an entire year of training behind them. Subjects referred to as in year 0 were tested two weeks after the start of their first year of training. This was the most practical way to get a given population of subjects together while minimizing the effect of prior training.

Table 1 Number of subjects by year and by curriculum
Table 2 Number of subjects by gender and by activity

Testing procedure

The test was done on paper, lasted for 35 minutes and was taken in the classroom by all students of the class at the same time. The teacher was present during the test. Participants were not allowed to communicate for the duration of the test. A timed PowerPoint presentation displayed the instructions and served as time keeper for the test to ensure equality of treatment among the subjects in the various classes. The instructions included sample questions with answers to ensure that participants understood the task. For the parts of the tests that had two series of questions (MR and PF), a 90 second break was given to the participants between the two series.

Scoring

Each correct answer was rewarded with one point. Each question in the PF and OP parts of the test had one correct answer and therefore yielded 0 or 1 point, while the MR questions each had a maximum of two correct answers, and therefore yielded 0, 1, or 2 points. The final score of the test was computed as an average of the percentage score of each part of the test, with an equal weight of one third for each of them:

score = 1 3 · S MR + 1 3 · S PF + 1 3 · S OP
(1)

where S denotes the percentage score of the given part of the test.

Data pre-processing

The data needed to be processed in two ways to allow comparison across the three populations. First, given the correlation between spatial skills and gender reported in the literature

girls were excluded from the data (in accordance with the literature, our results showed a significant gender difference in favor of males (F[1,724] =24.1, p=.000)).

Second, only carpenters had data for the third year. To avoid a potential year effect, the third year carpenters were removed from the data for the population comparison. They will be reintroduced when comparing the progression over time within the carpenter sample in the second part of the results section presented now.

Results and discussion

Results on H A : Population differences

Comparing populations on the overall score

A comparison of the overall score of the three populations with all males enrolled in year 0 to 2 showed no significant difference between the carpenters and the high school students, but revealed a significant difference between the logisticians and the rest of the subjects (F[2,560] =56.23, p=.000), as shown in Figure 4.

Figure 4
figure 4

Population comparison. Overall score comparison between carpenter apprentices, logistics apprentices, and high school students.

Spatial skills are often partially correlated with the general school level, and the general school level of carpenter apprentices is admittedly lower than that of high school students and comparable to that of logisticians (Stalder [30]). This means that carpenters’ spatial skills should be close to those of logisticians, but lower than that of high school students.

However, as can be seen in Figure 4, the carpenter’s performance is significantly higher than the logisticians’ and identical to the high school students’, suggesting that carpenters apprentices’ spatial skills are higher than expected.

Noteworthy is the fact that the spatial skills of carpenters were already strong at the beginning of their training. In particular, as can be seen in Figure 4, their performance in year 0 was similar to that of high school students and significantly higher than that of logisticians (F[1,110] =9.6, p=.002). This indicates that there is a selection effect prior to the start of the apprenticeship, be it a self-selection or an employer-motivated selection effect.

Comparing populations for each part of the test separately

We now look at the results for each sub-part of the test (Figure 5), as it can indicate where the difference of performance between the populations came from. The carpenters significantly outperformed the logisticians in all parts of the test. The high school students’ and the carpenters’ scores are close for the paper folding and the orthographic projection parts, but the carpenters’ score is significantly higher for the mental rotation part (F[1,446] =6.42, p=.01).

Figure 5
figure 5

Score by population. Score by population for each part of the tests.

Comparison with previous research

Comparing the carpenters’ scores with previous results reported in the literature can give an indication of the carpenters’ performance. There are many studies in which scores on the mental rotation test have been reported, almost all of them with undergraduate students as participants. For paper folding, there is a greater variety of tests, and therefore fewer studies with which the results of the current study can be compared. Moreover, some studies do not report the raw results required for a statistical comparison (mean and standard deviation, separated by gender). In the end, we selected three studies for mental rotation and two for paper folding. The tests used in the selected studies were the same as those used in the current study.

For mental rotation, Peters et al. ([22]) tested 237 male Bachelor students, 102 in Arts, and 135 in Science, with an average age of 21.3. Art students scored 50.4% (standard deviation (SD): 20.0%) on average, and science students got a mean score of 61.7% (SD: 20.0%). In comparison, the 439 male carpenter apprentices tested in the current study got a mean score of 58.1% (SD: 19.8%). According to a t-test, the carpenters’ performance is significantly higher than the Arts students’ (t[539] =−3.53, p=.000), but not significantly different from the Science students’ (t[572] =1.84, p =.66). Another study involved 139 male introductory psychology students (Voyer and Saunders [32]). The mean age of the participants was 19.8, and their average score, 60.7% (SD: 18.9%), which is not significantly different from the carpenters’ performance (t[576] =1.36, p =.17). A third study (Gouchie and Kimura [10]) tested 42 volunteer males (mean age 21.0), mostly undergraduate students. They scored 45.8% (SD: 39.0%), which is significantly lower than the carpenters’ score (t[479] =−3.44, p =.001).

In the same study (Gouchie and Kimura [10]), the participants also took the paper folding test (although they used only one of the two parts of the test used in the current study, i.e. 10 questions instead of 20). Their performance (mean =62.9%, SD =20.8%) was lower than the carpenters’ (mean =66.4%, SD =14.6%), although not significantly (t[479] =−1.42, p =.16). Kimura ([17]) measured the performance of 24 undergraduate male students (no mention of age) to 66% (SD: 24.0%), again using only 10 questions of the paper folding test. The performance was not significantly different from the carpenters’ performance (t[545] =−.22, p =.83).

These comparisons with previous research show that the carpenter apprentices’ performance on either the mental rotation or the paper folding test was as high, and even higher in some cases, than first year university students’ performance. This result goes along the same line the one above that showed that carpenter apprentices’ performance was similar to that of high school students and significantly higher than that of logistics apprentices. Together, these two results confirm the first hypothesis (H A ): The spatial skills of carpenter apprentices are higher than those of same age individuals that do not attend a carpentry training.

We now turn to the second hypothesis (H B ), which can be reformulated as a question: are these skills high by nature, or do they develop over the course of the apprenticeship?

Results on H B : Improvement over time

One way to look at the progression over time is to compare the performance of the apprentices in year 0 with the performance of the apprentices in year 3. However, this makes the assumption that, on average, each yearly batch of apprentices started their apprenticeship with the same spatial skills level. Another way to measure the progression over time that gets rid of this assumption is to measure the actual skill improvement over time by testing the same apprentices twice: once at the beginning of their apprenticeship, once towards the end of it. The results of both approaches are presented below.

Different students on different years

The 439 male carpenters were spread over the 4 years (see Table 1). Figure 6 shows that carpenters tend to improve slightly over the course of their apprenticeship. According to a linear model fitted by regression (shown in the Figure as a line), the average score improvement for each year of training was 2.2%, which is a significant improvement (F[3,437] =4.65, p =.003). Although the 2.2% average improvement per year is statistically significant, comparing the results of the years two by two with a pairwise t-test tells a slightly different story. Indeed, while the score for year 0 is significantly different from that of the three other years, the year-to-year comparison of the performance in year 1, 2, and 3 shows no statistical differences.

Figure 6
figure 6

Score by year. The overall score for carpenter participants split by year.

This is a contrasted result: on the one hand there is a significant improvement over the four years tested, but on the other hand the improvement seems to come mostly from the lower result of participants tested in year 0. This could indicate that spatial skills improve strongly during the first year of the apprenticeship, and then only marginally. It could also be due to the drastic selection that occurs in the first year of the carpenters’ training, where about one third of the apprentices fail. The significant improvement over time found for carpenters does not appear for the two other populations (F[1,72] =2.62, p =0.11 for high school students, F[1,113] =0.44, p =0.50 for logisticians), although the non significance of the statistical result might also be due to the smaller sample size of those two groups.

Same students two years apart

A second way to measure whether the spatial skills level of carpenter apprentices improves during their apprenticeship is to test them twice during their apprenticeship: once at the beginning, and once towards the end of it. In the spring of 2012, two years after the first round of tests, a subset of the same carpenter apprentices – those who were still doing their apprenticeship – were administered the same test a second time.

Data

In total, 120 male apprentices took the test twice. In order to be able to make cleaner comparisons, 9 apprentices who had repeated a year were removed from the data set. The final sample size was 111, with 41 apprentices in year 0 in 2010 and in year 2 in 2012, and 70 apprentices in year 1 in 2010 and year 3 in the 2012. In the rest of this section, those two groups are refered to by the year in which they were enrolled in in 2010, i.e. year 0 and year 1.

Results

Table 3 shows the 2010 and 2012 performance for three different groups: all students together, only year 0 students, and only year 1 students. For each of these groups, the details for each part of the test are also shown. On top of the percentage scores of 2010 and 2012, the table shows the relative learning gain (RLG) between 2010 and 2012, and the statistical significance of the difference between the two scores, computed with a two-sample paired t-test. The RLG was computed as shown in Equation 1, where pre and post are the percentage scores for the test in 2010 and 2012, respectively.

RLG= 100 × post pre 100 pre , if ( post pre ) > 0 100 × post pre pre , if ( post pre ) 0
(2)
Table 3 Percentage scores for 2010 and 2012, RLG and statistical significance of the difference between the two years, separated by groups of students and by the parts of the test

There are other ways to report learning gain (score difference, percentage increase, etc.), but the RLG has the advantage of not penalizing students who scored well on the pre-test, because it measures the improvement achieved on the possible improvement from the pre-test score.

Improvement over the two years

There was a significant and positive global RLG between the 2010 and the 2012 scores (26.4%). The mean percentage score went from 62.8% to 72.6% (F[1,110] =95.78, p =.000). Even when looking at the parts of the test separately, each of them displays a strong and statistically significant improvement when measured on all students. The improvement was stronger than for the results reported in Figure 6: it went from a 2.2% average yearly improvement to a 4.9% average yearly improvement. This could be the result of the selection effect incurred by the removal of students redoing a year from the second set of data. Indeed, when taking into account only the years 0, 1, and 2 from the results in Figure 6, the yearly improvement goes up from 2.2% to 3.6% (57.5% in year 0 to 64.6% in year 2).

Improvement for each part of the test

When further splitting the students per year, the only non-significant result is the orthographic projection part for the year 0 group. In parallel, on the same part of the test, year 1 apprentices did improve significantly. This suggests that the improvement on the carpentry specific part is linked to the second and third year of the apprenticeship, rather than the first year of it. It is surprising, because what was tested in the carpentry part (orthographic projections) is already taught in the first year.

There are several explanations for this. One is that apprentices need some time to master orthographic projections, and longer exposure to them, both at work and at school, increases their performance. The fact that the performance on the other parts of the test improved could reflect the higher degree of specialized knowledge required for the orthographic projections. By comparison, the MR and PF tasks are purely spatial reasoning tasks and do not require extra knowledge. Another reason could be that the test did not capture improvement as well as the MR and PF parts, which had more questions and which benefited from more effort and meticulousness from psychologists in the design of the questions. Last but not least, the similarity between the OP part and the school exercises might have reduced the apprentices’ motivation.

Limitations of the current study

As already mentioned, measuring the progression by comparing different students enrolled in different years of the apprenticeship makes the assumption that the four batches of students started their apprenticeship with similar spatial skills. This assumption, although reasonable, cannot be verified. When measuring the progression in terms of score improvements of the same students within two years, there is a non measurable retest effect. Arguably, the retest effect two years apart is weak, but ideally, there should have been a control group to measure this retest effect.

Everything was done to ensure that the test conditions were the same for all subjects, especially time-wise, but practical constraints made it impossible to have the exact same conditions for all subjects. Subjects were tested on different days of the week, at different times, and with different surrounding school environments.

In the design of the test, a set of questions specific to the carpenters’ curriculum (OP) was added to two widely used tests (MR and PF). In order to keep the total duration of the test under the duration of a lesson, the number of questions in the OP part was limited to six. Ideally, to be able to pinpoint exactly what difficulties participants had with orthographic projections, the OP part should have comprised about 20 questions. However, this would have pushed the test over the duration of a lesson, which would in turn have made the administration of the test difficult. The information brought by the OP part is therefore limited.

Finally, the way in which the score was computed could be discussed. For instance, no difference was made between a wrong answer and a missing answer, as it is sometimes the case in the literature. Also, the final score was computed as a weighted average of each of the three parts, with each of the three weights being equal. Other ways to compute the final score would have been to use weights proportional to the number of questions or to the time allotted to complete a specific part of the test.

Despite its limitations, this study fulfilled its goals by providing useful information on carpenters’ spatial skills. In particular, it makes it possible to answer the two questions that were raised at the beginning of this section: whether carpenters have better spatial skills than other similar populations, and whether their spatial skills improve over the course of their apprenticeship.

Conclusions

Carpenters’ spatial skills are higher than would be expected...

The first hypothesis (H A ) was that carpenters had higher spatial skills than similar populations. This hypothesis was verified in two ways: through a first-hand comparison with high school students and logistics apprentices, and through a comparison with previously reported results of university students from the literature. The difference was found for different types of spatial tasks, i.e. mental rotation, paper folding, and orthographic projections. The results further showed that carpenter apprentices’ spatial skills were already high at the beginning of their apprenticeship, indicating that there is a selection bias in the way carpenters are chosen or choose themselves their profession.

... and they improve

The second hypothesis (H B ) was that carpenters’ spatial skills improved over the course of their apprenticeship. The measurements were done in two ways, but showed similar results: carpenter apprentices’ spatial skills performance increases over the years of the apprenticeship, and especially during the first year of their apprenticeship. We can therefore accept H B , with the reservation that part of this improvement may be due to a selection bias (weaker students leave). Interestingly, while the performance on orthographic projections globally improved, it did not improve during the first two years, although orthographic projections are mostly taught during the first year at school. This suggests that the way orthographic projections are taught to carpenter apprentices can be improved.

Implications for future work

The verification of the two hypotheses complements the results of the contextual inquiry, which established that spatial skills are key for carpenters. The importance of spatial skills is evident in the selection process – whether it is a self-selection or a company-based selection – as well as in the training received by the apprentices. As spatial skills are crucial in this profession, there is a need to develop further solutions that encourage further improvement of teaching and learning activities for spatial skills.

One of these solutions are TUIs, a special kind of computer interfaces that allow the user to control the computer through the manipulation of physical objects. TUIs allow apprentices to learn with concrete learning material and hands-on activities, in a similar way as they currently do, and our first results using such technology are promising (Cuendet et al. [3]; Cuendet et al. [4]; Cuendet and Dillenbourg [5]).

References

  1. Arnheim R: A plea for visual thinking. Empirical Research in Vocational Education and Training 1980, 6(3):489–497.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Carter C, Larussa M, Bodner G: A study of two measures of spatial ability as predictors of success in different levels of general chemistry. J Res Sci Teach 1987, 24(7):645–57. 10.1002/tea.3660240705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Cuendet S, Bumbacher EW, Dillenbourg P: Tangible vs. virtual representations: when tangibles benefit the training of spatial Skills. In Proc. NordiCHI 2012. ACM, New York, NY, USA; 2012a.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Cuendet S, Jermann P, Dillenbourg P: Tangible interfaces: when physical-virtual coupling may be detrimental to learning. In Proc. British HCI 2012. British Computer Society, Swinton, UK, UK; 2012b.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Cuendet S, Dillenbourg P (2013) The benefits and limitations of distributing a tangible interface in a classroom. In: Proc. CSCL 2013.. International Society of the Learning Sciences. Best paper award.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Do-Lenh S, Jermann P, Cuendet S, Zufferey G, Dillenbourg P: Task performance vs. learning outcomes: a study of a tangible user interface in the classroom. In Proc. EC-TEL 2010. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg; 2010:78–92.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Ekstrom RB, French JW, Harman HH, Dermen D: Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Educational Testing Service, Princeton, New Jersey; 1976.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Gaillard L (2012) Perspectives de la formation - scénarios 2011–2020 pour le degré secondaire ii. Technical report, Office fédéral de la formation professionnelle et de la technologie OFFT.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Gardner H: Multiple intelligences: new horizons. Basic Books, New York; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Gouchie C, Kimura D: The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1991, 16(4):323–334. 10.1016/0306-4530(91)90018-O

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hambrick D, Libarkin J, Petcovic H, Baker K, Elkins J, Callahan C, Turner S, Rench T, Ladue N: A test of the circumvention-of-limits hypothesis in scientific problem solving: The case of geological bedrock mapping. J Exp Psychol Gen 2011, Vol 141(3):397–403. 10.1037/a0025927

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hamlin AJ, Boermsa N, Sorby SA (2006) Do Spatial Abilities Impact the Learning of 3-D solid Modeling Software?Proceedings of the Annual Conference of ASEE, Chicago, IL, CD-ROM.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Hegarty M, Keehner M, Khooshabeh P, Montello DR: How spatial abilities enhance, and are enhanced by, dental education. Learn Individ Differences 2009, 19(1):61–70. 10.1016/j.lindif.2008.04.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hegarty M, Montello DR, Richardson AE, Ishikawa T, Lovelace K: Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence 2006, 34(2):151–176. 10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Ishii H, Ullmer B: Tangible bits: Towards seamless interfaces between people, bits and atoms. In CHI ’97. ACM, New York, NY, USA; 1997:234–241.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Jurdak M, Shahin I: Problem solving activity in the workplace and the school: the case of constructing solids. Educ Stud Math 2001, 47(3):297–315. 10.1023/A:1015106804646

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Kimura D: Body asymmetry and intellectual pattern. Pers Individ Differences 1994, 17(1):53–60. 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90261-5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Lafue G: Recognition of three-dimensional objects from orthographic views. In Proc. SIGGRAPH 1976, vol 10. ACM, New York, NY, USA; 1976:103–108.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Maier PH: Räumliches Vorstellungsvermögenn. Peter Lang Frankfurt, Frankfurt; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Manches A, O’Malley C: Tangibles for learning: a representational analysis of physical manipulation. Pers Ubiquitous Comput 2012, 16(4):405–419. 10.1007/s00779-011-0406-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Newcombe N (2010) Picture this: increasing math and science learning by improving spatial thinking. Am Educ43: 29–35.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Peters M, Laeng B, Latham K, Jackson M, Zaiyouna R, Richardson C: A redrawn vandenberg and kuse mental rotations test: different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain Cognition 1995, 28(1):39–58. 10.1006/brcg.1995.1032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Quarles J, Lampotang S, Fischler I, Fishwick P, Lok B: Tangible user interfaces compensate for low spatial cognition. In IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces. IEEE, Reno, Nevada, USA; 2008:11–18.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Shea DL, Lubinski D, Benbow CP 2001. Importance of assessing spatial ability in intellectually talented young adolescents: a 20-year longitudinal study, Vol. 93.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Sims V, Mayer R: Domain specificity of spatial expertise: the case of video game players. Appl Cognit Psychol 2002, 16(1):97–115. 10.1002/acp.759

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Smith IM: Spatial ability: its educational and social significance. University of London Press, London; 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Sommer R: The mind’s eye: imagery in everyday life. Delacorte Press, U.S.; 1978.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Sorby S: Educational research in developing 3-d spatial skills for engineering students. Int J Sci Educ 2009, 31(3):459–480. 10.1080/09500690802595839

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Sorby S, Casey B, Veurink N, Dulaney A (2013) The role of spatial training in improving spatial and calculus performance in engineering students. Learn Individ Differences26: 20–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Stalder B (2011) Le niveau d’exigences intellectuelles des formations professionnelles initiales en suisse. classement des années 1999 à 2005. Technical report, Université de Bâle /TREE.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Uttal DH, Meadow NG, Tipton E, Hand LL, Alden AR, Warren C, Newcombe NS: The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychol Bull 2012, 139(2):352–402. 10.1037/a0028446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Voyer D, Saunders KA: Gender differences on the mental rotations test: a factor analysis. Acta Psychol 2004, 117(1):79–94. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.05.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP: Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J Educ Psychol 2009, 101(4):817–835. 10.1037/a0016127

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sebastien Cuendet.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

SC and PD designed the tests. SC, EB, and CA carried out the studies in the classrooms. SC and EB carried out the statistical analysis. SC wrote the article with the help of JD. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits use, duplication, adaptation, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cuendet, S., Dehler-Zufferey, J., Arn, C. et al. A study of carpenter apprentices’ spatial skills. Empirical Res Voc Ed Train 6, 3 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40461-014-0003-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40461-014-0003-3

Keywords